Appendix C: Articles of interest to CAT4 users
The first two articles in this Appendix (Appendix C) illustrate the relevance of spatial testing, thereby highlighting the importance of recognition and testing of spatial intelligence in assessing students’ development in CAT4. The third looks at analysing CAT4 data in an English as an additional language (EAL) context.
Recognising Spatial Intelligence
Our schools, and our society, must do more to recognize spatial reasoning, a key kind of intelligence
By Gregory Park, David Lubinski and Camilla P. Benbow, Scientific American, November 2, 2010 online
Ninety years ago, Stanford psychologist Lewis Terman began an ambitious search for the brightest kids in California, administering IQ tests to several thousand of children across the state. Those scoring above an IQ of 135 (approximately the top 1 percent of scores) were tracked for further study. There were two young boys, Luis Alvarez and William Shockley, who were among the many who took Terman’s tests but missed the cutoff score. Despite their exclusion from a study of young “geniuses”, both went on to study physics, earn PhDs, and win the Nobel prize.
How could these two minds, both with great potential for scientific innovation, slip under the radar of IQ tests? One explanation is that many items on Terman’s Stanford-Binet IQ test, as with many modern assessments, fail to tap into a cognitive ability known as spatial ability. Recent research on cognitive abilities is reinforcing what some psychologists suggested decades ago: spatial ability, also known as spatial visualization, plays a critical role in engineering and scientific disciplines. Yet more verbally loaded IQ tests, as well as many popular standardized tests used today, do not adequately measure this trait, especially in those who are most gifted with it.
Spatial ability, defined by a capacity for mentally generating, rotating, and transforming visual images, is one of the three specific cognitive abilities most important for developing expertise in learning and work settings. Two of these, quantitative and verbal ability, are quite familiar due to their high visibility in standardized tests like the Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT). A spatial ability assessment may include items involving mentally rotating an abstract image or reasoning about how an illustrated mechanical device functions. All three abilities are positively correlated, such that someone with above average quantitative ability also tends to have above average verbal and spatial ability. However, the relative balance of specific abilities can vary greatly between individuals. While those with verbal and quantitative strengths have opportunities to be identified by standardized tests or school performance, someone with particularly strong spatial abilities can go unrecognized through these traditional means.
A recent review, published in the Journal of Educational Psychology, analyzed data from two large longitudinal studies. Duke University’s Jonathan Wai worked with two of us (Lubinski and Benbow) and showed how neglecting spatial abilities could have widespread consequences. In both studies, participants’ spatial abilities, along with many others, were measured in adolescence. The participants with relatively strong spatial abilities tended to gravitate towards, and excel in, scientific and technical fields such as the physical sciences, engineering, mathematics, and computer science. Surprisingly, this was after accounting for quantitative and verbal abilities, which have long been known to be predictive of educational and occupational outcomes. In a time when educators and policy-makers are under pressure to increase the number of students entering these fields, incorporating knowledge of spatial ability into current practices in education and talent searches may be the key to improving such efforts.
The first source of data reviewed by Wai was a massive longitudinal study, Project Talent. While several studies have investigated the role of spatial abilities in tasks involving visual searching or path finding, Wai and colleagues focused on the relationship between spatial abilities and interests, finding that adolescents with strong spatial abilities also show greater interest than most in working with their hands, manipulating and tinkering with tangible things. While building, repairing, and working with inanimate objects might bore some, spatially gifted adolescents reported a preference for such activities. When those same individuals were contacted again in their late 20s, they had pursued and persisted in scientific and technical fields, earning bachelor’s, Master’s and doctoral degrees in these areas at higher rates than their peers. These findings suggest that the same child who likes to dismantle and reassemble old electronics may be particularly well-suited for doing the same in adulthood with electrons, molecules, or microchips.
While those with verbal and quantitative strengths enjoy reading, writing, and mathematics classes, there are currently few opportunities in the traditional high school to discover spatial strengths and interests. Instead, students who might benefit from hands-on, technical material must find an outlet on their own time, or just wait until their post- secondary education. And, in the worst case, they may drop out of the educational system altogether.
The second source of data reviewed by Wai came from a large- scale talent search. Talent searches, similar to Terman’s project, use psychometric assessments to identify youths with exceptional talents, usually in quantitative or verbal ability, that might not be recognized in a traditional classroom setting. One of the goals of modern talent searches is to provide the additional educational opportunities and experiences needed by these students for optimal development. Adolescents with exceptionally high quantitative ability, for example, can benefit greatly by additional instruction or an accelerated mathematics curriculum that provides them with developmentally appropriate material, such as advanced calculus rather than algebra. When youths identified by talent searches are appropriately accelerated according to their intellectual strengths, they report higher satisfaction with their education as adults.
The talent search data reviewed by Wai was collected from the Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth (SMPY), a talent search initiated at Johns Hopkins University in the early 1970s. SMPY identified intellectually precocious adolescents at or before age 13 based on scores on the quantitative and verbal subtests of the SAT. After identification, many of these same adolescents were administered measures of spatial ability. Although these participants were selected based on their exceptional quantitative and verbal ability, there was wide variability in the spatial abilities within the sample.
These participants have now been followed for over 25 years, and the variability in spatial abilities was found to be predictive of educational and occupational outcomes, even after accounting for verbal and quantitative abilities. Similar to the subjects from Project Talent, the SMPY participants who earned bachelors, Master’s, and doctoral degrees in science and engineering fields had especially strong spatial abilities compared to the rest of the sample. The same trend was found among those who had occupations in these fields at age 33.
Due to the neglect of spatial ability in school curricula, traditional standardized assessments, and in national talent searches, those with relative spatial strengths across the entire range of ability constitute an under-served population with potential to bolster the current scientific and technical workforce. Alvarez and Shockley found their way despite being missed by the Terman search, and each had considerable impact on technology in the last century. But how many more Alvarezes and Shockleys have we missed? Given the potential of scientific innovations to improve almost all aspects of modern life, missing just one is probably one too many.
About the author(s)
Gregory Park is a PhD student in the Department of Psychology and Human Development at Vanderbilt University. David Lubinski is professor of psychology and co-director of the Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth (SMPY) at Vanderbilt University. Camilla P. Benbow is Patricia and Rodes Hart Dean of Peabody College of Education and Human Development and co-director of SMPY at Vanderbilt University.
Reproduced with permission. Copyright ©2010 Scientific American, a division of Nature America, Inc. All rights reserved.
Picture This: increasing Maths and Science Leraning by Improving Spatial Thinking
The following article has been slightly adapted for a UK audience.
By Nora S. Newcombe
Nora S. Newcombe is a professor of psychology at Temple University and the principal investigator of the Spatial Intelligence and Learning Center (which is funded by the National Science Foundation). She has been a visiting professor at the University of Pennsylvania, Princeton University, and the Wissenschaftskolleg in Berlin. She is also a past president of the Developmental Psychology division of the American Psychological Association.
Albert Einstein’s scientific accomplishments so impressed the world that his name is shorthand for intelligence, insight, and creativity. To be an Einstein is to be inconceivably brilliant, especially in maths and science. Yet Albert Einstein was famously late to talk, and he described his thinking processes as primarily non-verbal. ‘The words or the language, as they are written or spoken, do not seem to play any role in my mechanism of thought,’ he once said. ‘[There are] more or less clear images’.1 Research on his brain, preserved after death, has seemed to support his claim of thinking in spatial images: Sandra Witelson, a neuroscientist in Canada, found that his parietal cortex, an area of the brain used for spatial and mathematical thinking, was unusually large and oddly configured,2 and likely supported him in imagining the universe in innovative ways.
Einstein was unique, but he certainly was not the only scientist to depend on his ability to think spatially. Watson and Crick’s discovery of the structure of DNA, for example, was centrally about fitting a three-dimensional spatial model to existing flat images of the molecule. The fact is, many people who work in the sciences rely on their ability to think spatially, even if they do not make grand discoveries. Geoscientists visualise the processes that affect the formation of the earth. Engineers anticipate how various forces may affect the design of a structure. And neurosurgeons draw on MRIs to visualise particular brain areas that may determine the outcome of a surgical procedure.
So, is spatial thinking really a key to science, technology, engineering, and mathematics – the so-called STEM disciplines? Yes. Scores of high quality studies conducted over the past 50 years indicate that spatial thinking is central to STEM success. One of the most important studies is called Project Talent; it followed approximately 400,000 people from their secondary school years in the late 1950s to today.3 It found that people who had high scores on spatial tests in secondary school were much more likely to major in STEM disciplines and go into STEM careers than those with lower scores, even after accounting for the fact that they tended to have higher verbal and mathematical scores as well. Similar results have been found in other longitudinal studies: one began in the 1970s and tracked the careers of a sample of gifted students first studied in their early years at secondary school4; another began in the 1980s with observing the block play of preschoolers and followed their mathematics learning through secondary school.5
In short, the relation between spatial thinking and STEM is a robust one, emerging for ordinary students and for gifted students, for men and for women, and for people who grew up during different historical periods. Spatial thinkers are likely to be more interested in science and maths than less spatial thinkers, and are more likely to be good enough at STEM research to get advanced degrees.
So, would early attention to developing children’s spatial thinking increase their achievement in maths and science, and even nudge them toward STEM careers? Recent research on teaching spatial thinking suggests the answer may be yes.
Tests of Spatial Thinking
The following four tests were used in the Project Talent study. Here, each is briefly described and a sample item is provided. Answers for the sample items are given at the end of the article.
Editors
1. Three-dimensional spatial visualization: Each problem in this test has a drawing of a flat piece of metal at the left. At the right are shown five objects, only one of which might be made by folding the flat piece of metal along the dotted lines. You are to pick out the one of these five objects which shows just how the piece of flat metal will look when it is folded at the dotted lines. When it is folded, no piece of metal overlaps any other piece or is enclosed inside the object.
2. Two-dimensional spatial visualization: In this test each problem has one drawing at the left and five similar drawings to the right of it, but only one of the five drawings on the right exactly matches the drawing at the left if you turn it around. The rest of the drawings are backward even when they are turned around. For each problem in this test, choose the one drawing which, when turned around or rotated, is exactly like the basic drawing at the left.
3. Mechanical reasoning: This is a test of your ability to understand mechanical ideas. You will have some diagrams or pictures with questions about them. For each problem, read the question, study the picture above it, and mark the letter of the answer on your answer sheet.
While wheel X turns rouXnd and round in the direction shown, wheel W turns
A. in direction A.
B. in direction B.
C. first in one direction and then in the other.
4. Abstract reasoning: Each item in this test consists of a set of figures arranged in a pattern, formed according to certain rules. In each problem you are to decide what figure belongs where the question mark is in the pattern... The items have different kinds of patterns and different rules by which the drawings change
Copyright © 2009 by the American Psychological Association. Reproduced with permission. Spatial ability for stem domains: aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance.
WAI, JONATHAN; LUBINSKI, DAVID; BENBOW, CAMILLA P. JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY. VOL 101 (4), NOV 2009, 817–835. DOI: 10.1037/A0016127. THE USE OF APA INFORMATION DOES NOT IMPLY ENDORSEMENT BY APA.
What Do We Mean by Spatial Thinking?
So far, we have been casual in using the term ‘spatial thinking.’ But what do we really mean by it? Spatial thinking concerns the locations of objects, their shapes, their relations to each other and the paths they take as they move. All of us think spatially in many everyday situations: when we consider rearranging the furniture in a room, when we assemble a bookcase using a diagram or when we relate a map to the road ahead of us. We also use spatial thinking to describe non- spatial situations, such as when we talk about being close to a goal or describe someone as an insider.
This general description is helpful but in conducting research, precise definitions are necessary. For the Project Talent study, spatial thinking was defined by the four tests used to assess it; a sample item from each of those four tests is shown in the box on page 13.6 The first test asks us to imagine folding a two-dimensional shape into a three-dimensional one. The second asks us to mentally rotate a two- dimensional shape. The third asks us to imagine mechanical motion. The fourth asks us to see spatial patterns and progressions.
Tests like these four have been around for a century or so, and they remain useful assessments of spatial ability. But they do not cover the full range of abilities that fall under the term ‘spatial thinking,’ so today’s researchers are working on developing new assessments. For example, one very different kind of spatial thinking involves navigating around the wider world. Many people think that, to get where we are heading, we need to be able to form a mental map of the environment.7 It appears that some of us are much better than others at forming these integrated representations.8 Spatial thinking of this kind may also be relevant to STEM success, but this idea has not yet been tested, largely because we lack good tests of navigation ability that can be given to large samples of students. Computer technology may soon allow such assessments.
To really understand what spatial thinking is, we must be clear about what it is not. First, spatial thinking is not a substitute for verbal or mathematical thinking. Those who succeed in STEM careers tend to be very good at all three kinds of thinking. Second, given the popularity of the notion that students have learning styles – i.e., that they are visual, auditory, or kinesthetic learners – it’s important to understand that spatial thinking is not a learning style. The truth is that there is virtually no support for learning styles in the research literature. While students may have preferences, all of us (with very rare exceptions) learn by seeing, hearing, and doing.* Likewise, all of us (with very rare exceptions) think verbally, mathematically, and spatially. So teachers should be trying to provide students with the content knowledge, experiences, and skills that support development of all three ways of thinking.
* Instead of tailoring lesson to students’ supposed learning styles, teachers should be concerned with tailoring their lessons to the content (e.g., showing pictures when studying art and reading aloud when studying poetry). For a thorough explanation of this, see ‘Do Visual, Auditory, and Kinesthetic Learners Need Visual, Auditory, and Kinesthetic Instruction?’ by Daniel T. Willingham in the Summer 2005 issue of American Educator, available at www.aft.org/newspubs/periodicals/ae/issues.cfm.
Can Spatial Thinking Actually Be Improved?
Since spatial thinking is associated with skill and interest in STEM fields (as well as in other areas, such as art, graphic design, and architecture), the immediate question is whether it can be improved. Can we educate children in a way that would maximise their potential in this domain? Americans often believe that their abilities are fixed, perhaps even at birth;9 it is not uncommon to hear that a person was born with a gift for mathematics or a difficulty in learning foreign languages. But there is mounting evidence that this is not the case.10 Abilities grow when students, their parents, and their teachers believe that achievement follows consistent hard work and when anxiety about certain areas, such as maths, is kept low.†
† Summing up 30 years of research, Daniel T. Willingham wrote, ‘Intelligence can be changed through sustained hard work.’ For his explanation of the genetic and environmental influences on intelligence, see the sidebar on page 10 of the Spring 2009 issue of American Educator, available at www.aft.org/ newspubs/periodicals/ae/issues.cfm.
What about spatial thinking in particular – is it malleable? Definitely. We have known for some time that primary school children’s spatial thinking improves more over the school year than over the summer months.11 A recent meta-analysis (which integrated the results of all the high quality studies of spatial malleability conducted over the past few decades) showed substantial improvements in spatial skill from a wide variety of interventions, including academic coursework, task-specific practice and playing computer games that require spatial thinking, such as Tetris (a game in which players rotate shapes to fit them together as they drop down the screen).12 Furthermore, these improvements were durable, and transferred to other tasks and settings. For example, when undergraduates were given extended, semester-long practice on mental rotation, through taking the test repeatedly and also through weekly play of Tetris, training effects were massive in size, lasted several months, and generalised to other spatial tasks such as constructing three-dimensional images from two-dimensional displays.13 Along similar lines, undergraduates who practised either mental rotation or paper folding daily, for three weeks, showed transfer of practice gains to novel test items, as well as transfer to the other spatial tasks they had not practised. 14 Spatial training has also been found to improve educational outcomes, such as helping college students complete engineering degrees.15
While many studies have found that spatial thinking can be improved, researchers have found some important differences between high and low ability participants. For low ability participants, there is an initial hump to get over. They improve slowly, if at all, for the first half-dozen or so sessions.** But if they persevere, faster improvement comes, so it’s important that students (and teachers) not give up.16 High ability participants do not have an initial hump, but they still can improve. Even people who are spatially proficient turn out to be not nearly as proficient as they could be, and they can attain even higher levels of excellence through fun activities like playing Tetris.17 While playing Tetris may not fit into the school day, it might be offered in after-school settings or be suggested to students as a weekend or summer activity (in moderation, of course). (Other spatial thinking activities that fit better into academic studies, such as why the earth has seasons, are discussed later.)
** [Researchers are not sure why this is. It could be that those who are not good at spatial thinking have not yet developed mental strategies for dealing with spatial problems. So, in the initial stage when it appears that they are not improving, they could be developing and testing strategies. Then, once they have hit on an effective strategy, they start to improve and continue improving as they practice. In contrast, high-ability participants already have effective mental strategies and are simply becoming better through practice.]
In addition to practising spatial thinking tasks like those shown in the box on pages 17-18, well-conceived symbolic representations, analogies and gestures are also effective in improving one’s spatial thinking ability. Let’s discuss each of these briefly.
One of the distinctive characteristics of human beings is that they can use symbolic representations, such as language, maps, diagrams, sketches, and graphs. Spatial language is a powerful tool for spatial learning. Babies learn a spatial relation better when it is given a name,18 preschoolers who understand spatial words like ‘middle’ perform better on spatial tasks than those who do not,19 and preschool children whose parents use a greater number of spatial words (like outside, inside, under, over, around, and corner) show better growth in spatial thinking than children whose parents do not use such language.20 Adults’ spatial thinking is also enhanced by spatial language (e.g., the word parallel helps pick out an important spatial concept), as is their thinking about concepts, such as time, that are often described with spatial metaphors (e.g., far in the future).21 Along similar lines, the ability to use maps can transform our thinking,22 allowing us to draw conclusions that would be hard to arrive at without maps. A famous example is seeing the relation between drinking polluted water and getting cholera; in the 1800s, a map of water pumps in London superimposed on a map of cholera cases made the case for a relationship. Like maps, diagrams, sketches and graphs also allow us to make inferences by supporting our spatial thinking.23 For example, a graph of how boys and girls change in height over childhood and adolescence shows us very clearly that, on average, girls have an earlier growth spurt and finish growing earlier.
In addition to being able to think symbolically, humans have a distinctive ability to think analogically, that is, to see relational similarities between one situation and another. People can learn through noticing analogies, that is, by comparing two situations and noting their common relational structure (as when we compare the structure of the atom to the structure of the solar system). This process facilitates learning in children,24 including spatial learning,25 mathematical insight,26 and scientific reasoning.27 Thus, an additional way to get children to develop spatial reasoning abilities is to point out and highlight key comparisons they should be making.
People also gesture as they think, and gesture has turned out to be not only a window into how thinking occurs,28 but also a powerful tool for improving various kinds of learning. Gestures provide a window into learners’ minds and offer information about whether a learner is ready to improve on a task.29 But gesture can also play a more active role in learning, in two ways. First, when teachers use gesture in instruction, children often learn better than when taught with speech alone.30 Second, when children gesture as they explain a problem, either prior to31 or during32 instruction, they learn better than if they do not gesture. Gesture is a powerful means of reflecting and communicating about spatial knowledge. Gesture has the potential to be a particularly powerful instructional tool in the spatial domain because it is particularly good at capturing spatial relationships among objects. For example, when talking about how the earth turns and revolves around the sun, teachers can gesture to capture those relationships.
Overall, our bag of tricks for enhancing spatial thinking is quite full. But there is more to learn. We know that practice, symbolic representations, analogies and gestures all improve spatial thinking, but we don’t know which of these approaches is most effective. Teachers will have to use their best judgment and fit spatial thinking into the school day as best they can. To help, I offer some suggestions at the end of this article.
What About Gender Differences?
Gender differences are often the first thing people want to talk about when they consider spatial thinking. Three big questions usually come to mind: Do gender differences exist? If so, how big are they? What causes them – are they biological or environmental? Research has found gender differences in spatial thinking ability, both among average men and women, and among the very highest achievers. For some spatial tests, these differences are large. However, while these differences do exist, we need to remember that average gender differences do not tell us about individual performance – some girls have strong spatial skills and some boys are lacking these skills. Gender differences in spatial thinking are no barrier to women’s success in the STEM disciplines as long as educators take the steps to ensure that all students, of both sexes, acquire the spatial thinking skills they need.
The question about causes is a tricky one. The assumption behind this question is usually that, if biological, the difference is immutable, whereas if environmental, it could be reduced or even eradicated. There are two problems with the question, however. The first problem is with the assumption behind it: biological causation does not imply immutability and environmental causation does not guarantee changeability. The second problem is that we don’t know the answer. A specially assembled team of experts with various takes on the problem recently concluded that there was evidence supporting both kinds of influences, with the additional possibility that the influences interacted (as when experience alters brain structures).33
Since spatial thinking can be improved, the important fact is not the causation of gender differences but the fact that girls (and boys) can improve. Some have suggested special training for females to help them catch up to males,34 but as educators we want all students to do their best. That means we may not close the gap: meta-analyses have found that the sexes generally improve in parallel and thus the gender difference continues even with training35 (although some exceptions have been reported in which performance by men and women converged36). Nevertheless, even if the gap does not close, many women (and men) can and will come to perform well above threshold levels for success in the STEM disciplines, at which point other factors such as persistence, communication and creativity may be more important than spatial ability.
What Does This Mean for Teachers?
Since spatial cognition is malleable, spatial thinking can be fostered with the right kind of instruction and technology. As we have seen, spatial thinking improves during the school year more than over the summer months,37 showing that teachers are helping students already. But what exactly should we be doing to help them improve even more? Unfortunately, precise answers are not yet possible. The National Academies’ report Learning to Think Spatially pointed out that we still lack specific knowledge of what kinds of experiences lead to improvement, how to infuse spatial thinking across the curriculum, or whether (and how best) to use new technologies such as Geographic Information Systems, especially with young children. What kinds of teaching best support spatial learning? Are these kinds of teaching different at different ages, at different socioeconomic status levels, or for girls and boys? Developing and testing curricula in a scientific way can be a slow process, and much remains to be done to be absolutely sure of our ground. However, we are beginning to have some good ideas about where to start, especially with preschool and primary school students.
1. Teachers (and parents) need to understand what spatial thinking is, and what kinds of pedagogical activities and materials support its development. Recall that spatial thinking involves noticing and remembering the locations of objects and their shapes and being able to mentally manipulate those shapes and track their paths as they move. Because spatial thinking is not a subject, not something in which children are explicitly tested, it often gets lost among reading, mathematics and all the other content and skills specified in state standards. Teachers need to be able to recognize where they can infuse it into the school day. For example, teachers could use the cardinal directions (north, south, east and west) to talk about how to get to the cafeteria or playground, or use words like parallel and perpendicular when possible.
2. Teachers at all levels need to avoid infusing students with anxiety about spatial tasks. In general, anxiety about doing a task can impede performance, at least in part by occupying valuable mental space in working memory.38 When you spend a lot of time worrying that you won’t do well, you lack the cognitive resources to actually concentrate on the work, a sad example of a self-fulfilling prophecy. Research with 6- to 8-year-olds in the Chicago Public Schools has recently shown that this vicious circle is evident for spatial thinking as well as for other areas like maths: children who worry about not doing well perform more poorly than children who do not have such anxiety.39 Thus, as is also true for other areas in teaching, teachers should avoid presenting spatial tasks as difficult challenges on which some people may not do well, or presenting students’ performance on these tasks as indicative of their underlying spatial abilities. Instead, teachers should emphasize that the tasks can be enjoyable and useful, and that they can be mastered with some effort and time.
3. In the preschool years, teachers (and parents) need to encourage, support and model engagement in age-appropriate spatial activities of a playful nature. Preschool children need a good balance of play and formal instruction.40 Fortunately, there is a wealth of spatial material available for preschool play, much of which can be further leveraged by a teacher with knowledge of the processes of spatial learning. Here are some specific ideas that could fit into most preschool settings:
- Select spatially challenging books for young children. For example, Zoom 41 is a book in which attention continually zooms in to finer and finer levels of detail. Verbal and gestural support for children in dealing with the book’s conceptual and graphic challenges is correlated with children’s scores on spatial tests.42
- Use odd-looking as well as standard examples when teaching the names of geometric shapes such as circle, square and triangle (e.g. a tipped, skinny, scalene triangle as well as an equilateral triangle pointing up). Showing these kinds of shapes supports learning that triangles are any closed figure formed by three intersecting straight lines.43
- Teach spatial words such as out, in, outside, inside, middle, between, here, there, front, back, side, top, bottom, up, down, under, over, around, tall, high, short, low, line (it) up, row, next (to) and corner. Learning spatial words can be enhanced by using gestures that highlight the spatial properties being discussed.44
- Encourage young children to gesture. Research has found that when children are asked whether two shapes can be fitted together to make another shape, they do significantly better when encouraged to move their hands to indicate the movements that would be made in pushing the shapes together.45 Some children do this spontaneously, but children who do not will perform better when asked to gesture.
- Ask children to imagine where things will go in simple ‘experiments’. For example, preschoolers are prone to think that dropped objects will appear directly below where they were released, even when they are dropped into a twisting tube with an exit point far away. But, when asked to visualise the path before responding, they do much better. Simply being asked to wait before answering does not help – visualization is key.46
- Do jigsaw puzzles with children; they have been found to predict good spatial thinking, especially when coupled with spatial language (e.g., Can you find all the pieces with a flat edge?).47 Similarly, play with blocks is a great activity in itself, and it increases use of spatial language.48
- Use maps and models of the world with children as young as 3.49
- Develop analogies to help young children learn scientific ideas, such as the principle of how a brace supports a building.50 Consider the two photos below. In the one on top, comparing the two structures is relatively easy because the only difference is whether the brace is diagonal or horizontal, but on the bottom the comparison is more difficult because the two structures differ in several ways. When children shake these structures to see how much they wiggle, they are much more likely to conclude that a diagonal piece increases stability when interacting with the display on top.
4. In the primary school years, teachers need to supplement the kinds of activities appropriate for preschoolers with more focused instruction in spatial thinking. Playful learning of the sort that occurs in preschool can continue to some extent in primary school; activities such as block building, gesturing, reading spatially challenging books, etc., continue to develop spatial skills in older children too.51 But as children get older, they can also benefit from more focused lessons. Mathematics is a central subject in which spatial thinking is needed, because space provides a concrete grounding for number ideas, as when we use a number line, use base-10 blocks, or represent multiplication as area. Here are some specific ideas for children in nursery through Y6/7:
- Highlight spatial elements in mathematics lessons. Measurement, for example, can be difficult for children to master, especially when the object to be measured is not aligned with the end of a ruler. Children often make mistakes such as counting hash marks beginning with 1, thus getting an answer that is one unit too many. When teaching measurement in early primary, teachers can consider using a technique in which the unit between hash marks on a ruler is highlighted as the unit of measurement.52 As shown in the illustration below, children can work with small unit markers coordinated with larger pieces to highlight how to determine units.
- Add mapping skills, when possible, to geography lesson for older primary students. Some ideas can be found in Phil Gersmehl’s book, Teaching Geography, which is based in part on cognitive science.53
- Use well-crafted analogies so that comparisons will highlight essential similarities and differences. For example, students can compare diagrams of animal and plant cells to see similarities and differences.54
- Ask children from around the ages of 9 to 14 to make sketches to elaborate on their understanding of topics such as states of matter, or force and motion.55 For example, they can be asked to draw water molecules in the form of ice, liquid, or vapor.
- Suggest beneficial recreational activities, such as photography lessons (to develop a sense of shifting viewpoints and changes in scale56), origami (to deepen their knowledge and skill in combining shapes) and JavaGami57 (software for creating polyhedra) and video games like Tetris.58
Spatial thinking is important, probably as important as verbal and mathematical thinking, for success in science, technology, engineering and mathematics. Furthermore, it can be taught and something we do in schools is already associated with improving it. Yet we can do better. The need to develop students’ spatial thinking is currently not widely understood. We already have some excellent techniques for developing it, through practice, language, gesture, maps, diagrams, sketching and analogy. Systematically building these techniques into the curriculum could yield important dividends for education.
Bibliography
1. Albert Einstein, ‘Letter to Jacques Hadamard,’ in The Creative Process: Reflections on Invention in the Arts and Sciences, ed. Brewster Ghiselin (Los Angeles: University of California Press, 1980).
2. Sandra F. Witelson, Debra L. Kigar, and Thomas Harvey, ‘The Exceptional Brain of Albert Einstein,’ Lancet 353, no. 9170 (June 1999): 2149–2153.
3. Jonathan Wai, David Lubinski, and Camilla P. Benbow, ‘Spatial Ability for STEM Domains: Aligning over 50 Years of Cumulative Psychological Knowledge Solidifies Its Importance,’ Journal of Educational Psychology 101, no. 4 (2009): 817–835.
4. Daniel L. Shea, David Lubinski, and Camilla P. Benbow, ‘Importance of Assessing Spatial Ability in Intellectually Talented Young Adolescents: A 20-Year Longitudinal Study,’ Journal of Educational Psychology 93, no. 3 (2001): 604–614.
5. Charles H. Wolfgang, Laura L. Stannard, and Ithel Jones, ‘Advanced Constructional Play with LEGOs among Preschoolers as a Predictor of Later School Achievement in Mathematics,’ Early Child Development and Care 173, no. 5 (2003): 467–475.
6. Wai, Lubinski, and Benbow, ‘Spatial Ability for STEM Domains.’
7. John O’Keefe and Lynn Nadal, The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map (Oxford, England: Oxford University Press, 1976).
8. Toru Ishikawa and Daniel R. Montello, ‘Spatial Knowledge Acquisition from Direct Experience in the Environment: Individual Differences in the Development of Metric Knowledge and the Integration of Separately Learned Places,’ Cognitive Psychology 52, no. 2 (2006): 93–129.
9. Carol S. Dweck and Ellen L. Leggett, ‘A Social-Cognitive Approach to Motivation and Personality,’ Psychological Review 95, no. 2 (1988): 256–273.
10. Stephen J. Ceci, ‘How Much Does Schooling Influence General Intelligence and Its Cognitive Components? A Reassessment of the Evidence,’ Developmental Psychology 27, no. 5 (1991): 703–722.
11. Janellen Huttenlocher, Susan Levine, and Jack Vevea, ‘Environmental Input and Cognitive Growth: A Study Using Time-Period Comparisons,’ Child Development 69, no. 4 (1998): 1012–1029.
12. Uttal, D. H., Meadow, N. G., Tipton, E., Hand, L. L., Alden, A. R., Warren, C., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013). The malleability of spatial skills: A meta-analysis of training studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139(2), 352-402. ‘Malleability of Spatial Cognition: A Meta-Analytic Review’ (under revision).
13. Melissa S. Terlecki, Nora S. Newcombe, and Michelle Little, ‘Durable and Generalized Effects of Spatial Experience on Mental Rotation: Gender Differences in Growth Patterns,’ Applied Cognitive Psychology 22, no. 7 (2008): 996–1013.
14. Rebecca Wright, William L. Thompson, Giorgio Ganis, Nora S. Newcombe, and Stephen M. Kosslyn, ‘Training Generalized Spatial Skills,’ Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 15, no. 4 (2008): 763–771
15. Sheryl Sorby, ‘Assessment of a ‘New and Improved’ Course for the Development of 3-D Spatial Skills,’ Engineering Design Graphics Journal 69, no. 3 (2005): 6–13.
16. Terlecki, Newcombe, and Little, ‘Durable and Generalized Effects.’
17. Terlecki, Newcombe, and Little, ‘Durable and Generalized Effects’; and Wight et al. Training Generalized spatial skills.’
18. Marianella Casasola, Jui Bhagwat, and Anne S. Burke, ‘Learning to Form a Spatial Category of Tight-Fit Relations: How Experience with a Label Can Give a Boost,’ Developmental Psychology 45, no. 3 (2009): 711–723.
19. Nina Simms and Dedre Gentner, ‘Helping Children Find the Middle: Spatial Language Facilitates Spatial Reasoning’ (poster presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Denver, April 2009).
20. Elizabeth Albro, Julie Booth, Susan Levine, and Christine Massey, ‘Making Cognitive Development Research Relevant in the Classroom’ (paper presented at the Cognitive Development Society Conference, San Antonio, TX, October 2009).
21. Lera Boroditsky, ‘Does Language Shape Thought? Mandarin and English Speakers’ Conceptions of Time,’ Cognitive Psychology 43, no. 1 (2001): 1–22; and Daniel Casasanto, ‘Similarity and Proximity: When Does Close in Space Mean Close in Mind?’ Memory and Cognition 36, no. 6 (2008): 1047–1056.
22. David H. Uttal, ‘Seeing the Big Picture: Map Use and the Development of Spatial Cognition,’ Developmental Science 3, no. 3 (2000): 247–264.
23. Barbara Tversky, ‘Spatial Schemas in Depictions,’ in Spatial Schemas and Abstract Thought, ed. Merideth Gattis (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2001).
24. Dedre Gentner, ‘Structure-Mapping: A Theoretical Framework for Analogy,’ Cognitive Science 7 (1983): 155–170; Dedre Gentner and Arthur B. Markman, ‘Structure Mapping in Analogy and Similarity,’ American Psychologist 52 (1997): 45–56; and Keith J. Holyoak, Dedre Gentner, and Boicho N. Kokinov, ‘The Place of Analogy in Cognition,’ in The Analogical Mind: Perspectives from Cognitive Science, ed. Dedre Gentner, Keith J. Holyoak, and Boicho N. Kokinov (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2001).
25. Stella Christie and Dedre Gentner, ‘Learning Novel Relations by Comparison,’ Journal of Cognition and Development (forthcoming); Laura Kotovsky and Dedre Gentner, ‘Comparison and Categorization in the Development of Relational Similarity,’ Child Development 67, no. 6 (1996): 2797–2822; Jeffrey Loewenstein and Dedre Gentner, ‘Spatial Mapping in Preschoolers: Close Comparisons Facilitate Far Mappings,’ Journal of Cognition and Development 2, no. 2 (2001): 189–219; and Jordan R. Vosmik and Clark C. Presson, ‘Children’s Response to Natural Map Misalignment during Wayfinding,’ Journal of Cognition and Development 5, no. 3 (2004): 317–336.
26. Bethany Rittle-Johnson and Jon R. Star, ‘Does Comparing Solution Methods Facilitate Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge? An Experimental Study on Learning to Solve Equations,’ Journal of Educational Psychology 99, no. 3 (2007): 561–574.
27. Zhe Chen and David Klahr, ‘All Other Things Being Equal: Acquisition and Transfer of the Control of Variables Strategy,’ Child Development 70, no. 5 (1999): 1098–1120; and Miriam Bassok and Keith J. Holyoak, ‘Interdomain Transfer between Isomorphic Topics in Algebra and Physics,’ Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 15, no. 1 (1989): 153–166.
28. R. Breckinridge Church and Susan Goldin-Meadow, ‘The Mismatch between Gesture and Speech as an Index of Transitional Knowledge,’ Cognition 23, no. 1 (1986): 43–71.
29. Susan Goldin-Meadow and Melissa A. Singer, ‘From Children’s Hands to Adults’ Ears: Gesture’s Role in the Learning Process,’ Developmental Psychology 39, no. 3 (2003): 509–520.
30. Melissa A. Singer and Susan Goldin-Meadow, ‘Children Learn When Their Teacher’s Gestures and Speech Differ,’ Psychological Science 16,no. 2 (2005): 85–89.
31. Sara C. Broaders, Susan Wagner Cook, Zachary Mitchell, and Susan Goldin-Meadow, ‘Making Children Gesture Brings Out Implicit Knowledge and Leads to Learning,’ Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 136, no. 4 (2007): 539–550.
32. Susan Wagner Cook, Zachary Mitchell, and Susan Goldin-Meadow, ‘Gesturing Makes Learning Last,’ Cognition 106, no. 2 (2008): 1047–1058; and Susan Goldin-Meadow, Susan Wagner Cook, and Zachary A. Mitchell, ‘Gesturing Gives Children New Ideas about Math,’ Psychological Science 20, no. 3 (2009): 267–272.
33. Diane F. Halpern, Camilla P. Benbow, David C. Geary, Ruben C. Gur, Janet Shibley Hyde, and Morton Ann Gernsbacher, ‘The Science of Sex Differences in Science and Mathematics,’ Psychological Science in the Public Interest 8, no. 1 (2007): 1–51.
34. Diane F. Halpern, Joshua Aronson, Nona Reimer, Sandra Simpkins, Jon R. Star, and Kathryn Wentzel, Encouraging Girls in Math and Science (Washington, DC: National Center for Education Research, Institute of Education Sciences, 2007).
35. Uttal DH, Meadow NG, Tipton E, Hand LL, Alden AR, Warren C, Newcombe NS. The malleability of spatial skills: a meta-analysis of training studies. Psychological Bulletin 139, no. 2 (2013): 352 - 402.
36. Ian Spence, Jingjie Jessica Yu, Jing Feng, and Jeff Marshman, ‘Women Match Men When Learning a Spatial Skill,’ Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 35, no. 4 (2009): 1097–1103.
37. Huttenlocher J, Levine S, Vevea J. Environmental input and cognitive growth: a study using time-period comparisons. Child Development 69, no. 4 (1998): 1012-1129.
38. Sian L. Beilock, ‘Math Performance in Stressful Situations,’ Current Directions in Psychological Science 17, no. 5 (2008): 339–343; and Sian L. Beilock, Elizabeth A. Gunderson, Gerardo Ramirez, and Susan C. Levine, ‘Female Teachers’ Math Anxiety Impacts Girls’ Math Achievement,’ Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (forthcoming) 107, no. 5 (2010): 1860-1863.
39. Gerardo Ramirez, Elizabeth A. Gunderson, Susan C. Levine, and Sian L. Beilock, ‘Spatial Ability, Spatial Anxiety, and Working Memory in Early Elementary School’ (paper presented in the Spatial Intelligence and Learning Center Showcase, October 2009).
40. Kathy Hirsh-Pasek, Laura E. Berk, Dorothy G. Singer, and Roberta M. Golinkoff, A Mandate for Playful Learning in Preschool: Presenting the Evidence (New York: Oxford University Press, 2008).
41. Zoom, Istvan Banyai, Viking Children’s Books, 1995.
42. Lisa E. Szechter and Lynn S. Liben, ‘Parental Guidance in Preschoolers’ Understanding of Spatial- Graphic Representations,’ Child Development 75, no. 3 (2004): 869–885.
43. Eric Satlow and Nora S. Newcombe, ‘When Is a Triangle Not a Triangle? Young Children’s Developing Concepts of Geometric Shape,’ Cognitive Development 13, no. 4 (1998): 547–559; and Kelly Fisher, Bertha Nash, Kathy Hirsh-Pasek, Nora S. Newcombe, and Roberta M. Golinkoff, ‘Breaking the Mold: Altering Preschoolers’ Concepts of Geometric Shapes’ (poster presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Denver, April 2009).
44. Erica Cartmill, Shannon M. Pruden, Susan C. Levine, and Susan Goldin-Meadow, ‘The Role of Parent Gesture in Children’s Spatial Language Development’ (presentation given at the 34th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, November 2009).
45. Stacy Ehrlich, Susan L. Levine, and Susan Goldin-Meadow, ‘Gestural Training Effects on Children’s Mental Rotation Skills’ (poster presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Denver, April 2009).
46. Amy Joh, Vikram Jaswal, and Rachel Keen, ‘Imagining a Way Out of the Gravity Bias: Preschoolers Can Visualize the Solution to a Spatial Problem,’ Child Development (forthcoming) 82, no. 3 (2011): 744-750.
47. Susan Levine, Kristin Ratliff, Janellen Huttenlocher, and Joanna Cannon, ‘Early Puzzle Play: A Predictor of Preschooler’s Mental Rotation Skill,’ Developmental Psychology (under review) 48, no. 2 (2012): 530-542.
48. Wendy L. Shallcross, Tilbe Göksun, Roberta Golinkoff, Kathy Hirsh-Pasek, Marianne E. Lloyd, Nora S. Newcombe, and Sarah Roseberry, ‘Building Talk: Parental Utterances during Construction Play’ (poster presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies, Vancouver, March 2008).
49. Judy S. DeLoache, ‘Young Children’s Understanding of Models,’ in Knowing and Remembering in Young Children, ed. Robyn Fivush and Judith A. Hudson (New York: Cambridge University Press, 1990), 94–126; and Anna Shusterman, Sang Ah Lee, and Elizabeth S. Spelke, ‘Young Children’s Spontaneous Use of Geometry in Maps,’ Developmental Science 11, no. 2 (2008): F1–F7.
50. Dedre Gentner, Susan Levine, Sonica Dhillon, and Ashley Poltermann, ‘Using Structural Alignment to Facilitate Learning of Spatial Concepts in an Informal Setting’ (paper presented at the 2nd International Analogy Conference, Sofia, Bulgaria, July 2009).
51. Beth M. Casey, Nicole Andrews, Holly Schindler, Joanne E. Kersh, Alexandra Samper, and Juanita Copley, ‘The Development of Spatial Skills through Interventions Involving Block Building Activities,’ Cognition and Instruction 26, no. 3 (2008): 269–309.
52. Susan C. Levine, Mee-kyoung Kwon, Janellen Huttenlocher, Kristin Ratliff, and Kevin Dietz, ‘Children’s Understanding of Ruler Measurement and Units of Measure: A Training Study,’ in Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. Niels A. Taatgen and Hedderik van Rijn (Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society, 2009), 2391–2395.
53. Phil Gersmehl, Teaching Geography (New York: Guilford Press, 2008).
54. See Rittle-Johnson and Star, ‘Does Comparing Solution Methods Facilitate Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge?’ Journal of Educational Psychology 99, no. 3 (2007): 561 - 574; and Jon R. Star and Bethany Rittle-Johnson, ‘It Pays to Compare: An Experimental Study on Computational Estimation,’ Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 102, no. 4 (2008): 408–426.
55. Nancy L. Stein, Florencia K. Anggoro, and Marc W. Hernandez, ‘Making the Invisible Visible: Conditions for the Early Learning of Science,’ in Developmental Cognitive Science Goes to School, ed. Nancy L. Stein and Stephen Raudenbush (New York: Taylor and Francis, forthcoming).
56. Lynn S. Liben and Lisa E. Szechter, ‘A Social Science of the Arts: An Emerging Organizational Initiative and an Illustrative Investigation of Photography,’ Qualitative Sociology 25, no. 3 (2002): 385–408.
57. Michael Eisenberg, Ann Eisenberg, Susan Hendrix, Glenn Blauvelt, Diana Butter, Jeremy Garcia, Ryan Lewis, and Tyler Nielsen, ‘As We May Print: New Directions in Output Devices and Computational Crafts for Children,’ in Proceedings of Interaction Design and Children 2003 (Preston, UK: ACM, 2003); and Michael Eisenberg, ‘Mindstuff: Educational Technology Beyond the Computer,’ Convergence 9, no. 2 (2003): 29–53.
58. Kaveri Subrahmanyam and Patricia M. Greenfield, ‘Effect of Video Game Practice on Spatial Skills in Girls and Boys,’ in Interacting with Video, vol. 11, ed. Patricia M. Greenfield and Rodney R. Cocking (Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation, 1996); and Jing Feng, Ian Spence, and Jay Pratt, ‘Playing an Action Video Game Reduces Gender Differences in Spatial Cognition,’ Psychological Science 18, no. 10 (2007): 850–855.
The Middle Child: Analysing Data in an EAL Context
By Nicola Lambros, Deputy Head, King’s College Madrid Published on: 29 Sep 2017
The importance of maintaining a focus on literacy within the curriculum has never been far away from the government’s agenda and anyone working within education would agree that developing strong literacy skills are key to a student’s success, particularly as external examinations consist of written papers.
Despite this, incorporating effective literacy strategies into a lesson can, at times, be challenging particularly if staff do not have clear data informing them of each student’s literacy capabilities. Furthermore, for some teachers, teaching literacy effectively within their lessons, especially those which are not literacy based, may not be an area of expertise. However, our classrooms are becoming progressively more globalised with increasing numbers of students having English as an Additional Language (EAL).
Some of these students are quickly identified for extra support as they present with very low levels of language acquisition; often these students are then tested further to establish specific areas of need and teachers are then provided with increased information and data to effectively differentiate their teaching which ensures these students make good progress. However, the majority of EAL students, in an international school environment, present with a good level of speaking and listening skills; they effectively communicate within the classroom and actively participate in learning activities. These students rarely raise concerns or are considered to be underachieving, particularly if their attitude to learning is good.
Should the Cognitive Abilities Test (CAT4) or a similar aptitude test, be completed these students will often sit within stanines 4-7 for their overall CAT4 score, results which are seen to confirm the fact that they are cognitively able and do not require extra support for literacy. Closer analysis of the CAT4 batteries can however reveal a very different picture.
Analysing CAT4 data from cohorts of primary and secondary students in two international schools in differing areas of the world, most if not all students with EAL have a significant verbal deficit (the difference between their standardised age score for the verbal and non-verbal batteries, any deficit larger than minus 10 being statistically significant). It is crucial that literacy development is a key focus in every lesson for students with a deficit of minus 10 or more if they are to achieve their very best across the curriculum. Therefore, every teacher must be or become a confident teacher of both their subject area and literacy, even if their subject is not literacy based.
When these students are further tested with the New Group Reading Test many of them often have good comprehension skills but significantly weaker word knowledge and vocabulary skills. This in practice means they can comprehend and rote learn information but lack the depth and breadth of vocabulary, in particular subject specific technical vocabulary, to explain in their own words what they have learned. This inhibits them from cognitively processing new information in a manner reflective of their non-verbal score which can reduce their ability to engage higher order thinking skills and therefore limit their progress and achievement. Furthermore, unless explicitly taught, grammar skills may also be lacking especially in older students who joined secondary school with little English.
Compounding these issues are the increasingly complex academic demands students face as they move through school and unless schools address the verbal deficit and close the literacy gap students with a verbal deficit will often struggle and underachieve. Notably, at first glance many of these students appear to be achieving good academic grades, but teachers should understand that if their verbal deficit is addressed much higher academic success is possible, particularly in the later stages of their education, university and beyond.
So what can we do? Very often it is as simple as making the implicit explicit. We need to explicitly teach literacy skills in context when the opportunity arises in the classroom. To name but a few:
- Consistently applying the school’s marking for literacy policy and giving students the opportunity to improve their writing when they have made mistakes;
- Explicitly teaching reading strategies such as skimming and scanning and taking time to teach students how to use diagrams, pictures, headings and topic sentences in text books to gather meaning and identify key points and ideas;
- Explicitly teaching writing strategies that are important for your subject such as effective note taking or writing a practical report in science;
- Always providing and referring to key word glossaries and giving opportunities for the use of technical language to be practiced;
- Scaffolding writing activities for students and incorporating opportunities to use writing strategies such as Point, Evidence, Explain wherever possible;
- Providing explicit success criteria for writing; presenting students with information in a variety of styles, e.g. research papers or more advanced text books and teaching information gathering strategies and encouraging the use of talk partners and providing scaffolds such as ‘Thought Stems’ to enable students to effectively discuss and clarify their ideas with a partner before writing them down.
If we provide teaching staff with key data with which to identify their students’ literacy needs and provide professional development to arm them with a number of tools to effectively teach literacy within all subjects, we can enable all teachers to become effective teachers of literacy. This, I believe, is one of the key components required to ensure every student realises their true potential and an important investment in the future of our young people.